When
trying to memorise new material, it’s easy to assume that the more work you put
in, the better you will perform. Yet taking the occasional down time – to do
literally nothing – may be exactly what you need. Just dim the lights, sit
back, and enjoy 10-15 minutes of quiet contemplation, and you’ll find that your
memory of the facts you have just learnt is far better than if you had
attempted to use that moment more productively.
Although
it’s already well known that we should pace our studies, new research suggests
that we should aim for “minimal interference” during these breaks –
deliberately avoiding any activity that could tamper with the delicate task of
memory formation. So no running errands, checking your emails, or surfing the
web on your smartphone. You really need to give your brain the chance for a
complete recharge with no distractions.
·
The
mystery of why you can’t remember being a baby
·
The
man who can’t trust his own brain
·
A
new way to master your emotions
An
excuse to do nothing may seem like a perfect mnemonic technique for the lazy
student, but this discovery may also offer some relief for people with amnesia
and some forms of dementia, suggesting new ways to release a latent, previously
unrecognised, capacity to learn and remember.
The
remarkable memory-boosting benefits of undisturbed rest were first documented
in 1900 by the German psychologist Georg Elias Muller and his student Alfons
Pilzecker. In one of their many experiments on memory consolidation, Muller and
Pilzecker first asked their participants to learn a list of meaningless
syllables. Following a short study period, half the group were immediately
given a second list to learn – while the rest were given a six-minute break
before continuing.
When
tested one-and-a-half-hours later, the two groups showed strikingly different
patterns of recall. The participants given the break remembered nearly 50% of
their list, compared to an average of 28% for the group who had been given no
time to recharge their mental batteries. The finding suggested that our memory
for new information is especially fragile just after it has first been encoded,
making it more susceptible to interference from new information.
Although
a handful of other psychologists occasionally returned to the finding, it was
only in the early 2000s that the broader implications of it started to become
known, with a pioneering study by Sergio Della Sala at the University of
Edinburgh and Nelson Cowan at the University of Missouri.
The team
was interested in discovering whether reduced interference might improve the
memories of people who had suffered a neurological injury, such as a stroke.
Using a similar set-up to Muller and Pilzecker’s original study, they presented
their participants with lists of 15 words and tested them 10 minutes later. In
some trials, the participants remained busy with some standard cognitive tests;
in others, they were asked to lie in a darkened room and avoid falling asleep.
The
impact of the small intervention was more profound than anyone might have
believed. Although the two most severely amnesic patients showed no benefit,
the others tripled the number of words they could remember – from 14% to 49%,
placing them almost within the range of healthy people with no neurological
damage.
The next
results were even more impressive. The participants were asked to listen to
some stories and answer questions an hour later. Without the chance to rest,
they could recall just 7% of the facts in the story; with the rest, this jumped
to 79% – an astronomical 11-fold increase in the information they retained. The
researchers also found a similar, though less pronounced, benefit for healthy
participants in each case, boosting recall between 10 and 30%.
Della
Sala and Cowan’s former student, Michaela Dewar at Heriot-Watt University, has
now led several follow-up studies, replicating the finding in many different
contexts. In healthy participants, they have found that these short periods of
rest can also improve our spatial memories, for instance – helping participants
to recall the location of different landmarks in a virtual reality environment.
Crucially, this advantage lingers a week after the original learning task, and
it seems to benefit young and old people alike. And besides the stroke survivors,
they have also found similar benefits for people in the earlier, milder stages
of Alzheimer’s disease.
In each
case, the researchers simply asked the participants to sit in a dim, quiet
room, without their mobile phones or similar distractions. “We don’t give them
any specific instructions with regards to what they should or shouldn’t do
while resting,” Dewar says. “But questionnaires completed at the end of our
experiments suggest that most people simply let their minds wander.”
Even
then, we should be careful not to exert ourselves too hard as we daydream. In
one study, for instance, participants were asked to imagine a past or future
event during their break, which appeared to reduce their later recall of the
newly learnt material. So it may be safest to avoid any concerted mental effort
during our down time.
No comments:
Post a Comment